The Influence of Absences of Caregiver Responsiveness in Social Interactions on the Development of Infant Social Bids
Main Article Content
Abstract
Social bids are any social behaviour an infant uses to capture their caregiver’s attention to engage in a social activity. Researchers have shown that infants learn to expect caregivers to respond to their social bids and that they may re-attempt a social bid when they do not receive the expected caregiver response. However, the influence of these moments of non-caregiver responsiveness on infant social bid development has been overlooked. In this thesis, I provide a longitudinal description that traces how these moments influence the diversity and amount of the social bids of two caregiver-infant dyads. I found, in line with past research, that infants will re-attempt the same or different social bid when their caregiver is unresponsive. To add, I found that infants increase the diversity and amount of their social bidding over time due to the events of caregiver non-responsiveness. Lastly, infants demonstrated highly idiosyncratic differences in their social bid presentations and changes in relation to caregiver non-responsiveness. These findings suggest that moments of caregiver non-responsiveness may play an important role in the development of infants’ social bid development, which predates their future language and social development.
Article Details
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
References
Albert, R. R., Schwade, J. A., & Goldstein, M. H. (2018). The social function of babbling:
Acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsiveness. Developmental Science, 18(11). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12641
Ardiel, E. L., & Rankin, C. H. (2010). The importance of touch in development. Paediatrics &
Child Health, 15(3), 153–156. https://doi.org/10.1093/pch/15.3.153
Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and
gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 1004. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.1004
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205-226. (Reprinted in E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin, Eds., Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1979, 111-128); reprinted in A. Kasher, Ed., Pragmatics: Critical concepts. London: Routledge, 1998, 274-295.
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of
symbols: Communication and cognition in infancy. New York, NY: Academic.
Butterworth, G. E., & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in
infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14, 219–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646839608404519
Bigelow, A. E., Power, M., Bulmer, M., & Gerrior, K. (2018). The effect of maternal mirroring
behavior on infants’ early social bidding during the still‐face task. Infancy, 23(3), 367–385. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1111/infa.12221
Bigelow, A. E., Power, M. (2022). Influences of infants’ and mothers’ contingent vocal
responsiveness on young infants’ vocal social bids in the Still Face Task. Infant Behavior and Development, 69, 101776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101776.
Blake, J., O’Rourke, P., & Borzellino, G. (1994). Form and function in the development of
pointing and reaching gestures. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 195-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(94)90055-8
Boundy, L., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Theakston, A. (2016). Exploring early communicative
behaviours: A fine-grained analysis of infant shows and gives. Infant Behavior & Development, 44, 86–97. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.005
Boundy, L., Cameron, F. T., & Theakston, A. (2019). Intention or attention before pointing: Do
infants’ early holdout gestures reflect evidence of a declarative motive? Infancy, 24(2), 228–248. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1111/infa.12267
Canfield, J. V. (1995). The rudiments of language. Language & Communication, 15(3), 195–211.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(95)00006-C
Carpendale, J. I. M., Atwood, S., & Kettner, V., (2013). Meaning and mind from the perspective
of dualist versus relational worldviews: Implications for the development of pointing gestures. Human Development, 56, 381–400. https://doi.org/10.1159/000357235
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Carpendale, A. B. (2010). The development of pointing: From personal
directedness to interpersonal direction. Human Development, 53, 110-126. https://doi.org/10.1159/000315168
Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The
development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 79–151. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000032
Carpenter, M. (2009). Just how joint is joint action in infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2),
–392. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01026.x
Cameron-Faulkner T, Theakston A, Lieven E, & Tomasello M (2015). The relationship between
infant holdout and gives, and pointing. Infancy, 20(5), 576–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12085
Cavell, T. A., & Quetsch, L. B. (2022). Good enough parenting: A six-point plan for a
stronger relationship with your child. American Psychological Association. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1037/0000324-000
Choi, B., Shah, P., Rowe, M. L., Nelson, C. A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2020). Gesture
development, caregiver responsiveness, and language and diagnostic outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 50(7), 2556–2572. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03980-8
Church, Sarah, Michael Dunn, and Linda Prokopy. 2019. Benefits to qualitative data quality
with multiple coders: Two case studies in multi-coder data analysis. Journal of Rural Social Sciences, 34(1). Article 2.
Collis, G. (1979). Describing the structure of social interaction in infancy. In M. Bullowa (Ed.),
Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for
developing grounded theory (3rd ed.). Sage Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452230153
Dahl, A. (2017). Ecological commitments: Why developmental science needs naturalistic
methods. Child Development Perspectives, 11(2), 79–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12217
Danziger, K. (1985). The methodological imperative in psychology. Philosophy of Social
Sciences, 15, 1–13.
Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In
C. Moore, & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
Esseily, R., Jacquet, A. Y., & Fagard, J. (2011). Handedness for grasping objects and pointing
and the development of language in 14‐month‐old infants. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 16, 565–585. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.499911
Fusaro, M. (2010). Toddlers get the nod: An analysis of “yes” and “no” gesture use during the
second to third year of life (Publication No. 3427125) [Doctural dissertation]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.
Fusaro, M., Vallotton, C. D., & Harris, P. L. (2014). Beside the point: Mothers’ head nodding and
shaking gestures during parent–child play. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(2), 235–247. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.01.006
Fusaro, M., & Vallotton, C. D. (2011). Giving a nod to social cognition: Developmental
constraints on the emergence of conventional gestures and infant signs. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 4, 121-135. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1075/gs.4.11fus
Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H.M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian
learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028044
Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveria, E. (2015).
Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1167. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167
Gros‐Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2014). Maternal responsiveness and the development
of directed vocalizing in social interactions. Infancy, 19(4), 385–408. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12054
Harding, C. G., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in
prelinguistic infants. Child Development, 50, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129038
Hsu, H.-C., Fogel, A., & Messinger, D. S. (2001). Infant non-distress vocalization during
mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Factors associated with quantitative and qualitative differences. Infant Behavior & Development, 24(1), 107–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00061-3
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in
infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/pap0000176
Jopling, D. (1993). Cognitive science, other minds, and the philosophy of dialogue. In U. Neisser
(Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 290–309). Cambridge: MIT Press.
O’Donnell, James J. “Augustine’s Classical Readings.” Recherches Augustiniennes, 1980,
–75. https://doi.org/10.5840/ancientphil199212239
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental
disorders. Trends Cognitive Science, 2, 389–398. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01230-3
Kettner, V.A., & Carpendale, J.I.M. (2018). From touching to communicating: Forms of index
finger use in the development of pointing. Gesture, 17(2), 245-268. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18005.ket
Kishimoto, T. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal observations of pointing gestures by
infants and their caregivers in Japan. Cognitive Development, 43, 235-244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.06.001
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict
toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1207–1219. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1037/a0024808
Kwon, A. Y., Vallotton, C. D., Kiegelmann, M., & Wilhelm, K. H., (2018). Cultural
diversification of communicative gestures through early childhood: A comparison of children in English- , German-, and Chinese- speaking families. Infant Behavior and Development, 50, 328-339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.10.003
Berends, L., & Johnston, J. (2005) Using multiple coders to enhance qualitative
analysis: The case of interviews with consumers of drug treatment. Addiction Research & Theory, 13(4), 373-381. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066350500102237
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C. (2012). A prelinguistic
gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x
Locke LM, Prinz RJ. Measurement of parental discipline and nurturance. Clinical Psychology
Review, 22, 895–929. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00133-2
Masataka, N. (2003). From index-finger extension to index-finger pointing: Ontogenesis of
pointing in preverbal infants. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 69-84). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Matsuki, T., Ebara, T., Tamada, H., Kato, S., Kaneko, K., Kano, H., Matsuzaki, K., Sato, H.,
Minato, K., Sugiura-Ogasawara, M., Saitoh, S., & Kamijima, M. (2022). Repeated maternal non-responsiveness to baby’s crying during postpartum and infant neuropsychological development: The Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 127, 1–15. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105581
Meeus, W. (2016). Adolescent psychosocial development: A review of longitudinal models and
research. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1969–1993. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000243
Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of
the Still-Face Paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29(2), 120–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001
Mcquaid, N. E., Bibok, M. B., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2009). Relation between maternal
contingent responsiveness and infant social expectations. Infancy, 14(3), 390–401. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000902839955
Moreno-Núøez A, Rodríguez C, & Miranda-Zapata E (2020). Getting away from the point: The
emergence of ostensive gestures and their functions. Journal of Child Language, 47(3), 556–578. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000606
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007).
Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x
Onishi, K., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science,
, 255– 258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621
Özçalışkan, Ş., Adamson, L. B., & Dimitrova, N. (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures
predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism, 20, 754–763. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315605921
Plato. (1901). Phaedo (B. Jowett, Trans.). In Plato & B. Jowett (Trans.), Dialogues of Plato: With
analyses and introductions. Charles Scribner's Sons. https://doi.org/10.1037/13728-011
Provenzi, L., Giusti, L., & Montirosso, R. (2016). Do infants exhibit significant cortisol
reactivity to the Face-to-Face Still-Face paradigm? A narrative review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 42, 34–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.07.001
Reddy, V. (2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 397–402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00191-8
Reddy, V. (2011). A gaze at grips with me. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New
developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience. MIT Press.
Reddy, V. (2018). Why Engagement? A Second-Person Take on Social Cognition. In A. Newen,
L. de Bruin, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Rips, L. J. (2002). Circular reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(6), 767–795.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00085-X
Rowe, M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low‐income caregivers and their 14‐month‐old
children. First Language, 20, 305–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237000200600
Rowe, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in
child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951-953. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167025
Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U. (2013). Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of
prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development, 84(4), 1296-1307. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12026
Scaife, M. & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature,
, 265-266. https://doi.org/10.1038/253265a0
Steinmetz, J., Xu, Q., Fishbach, A., & Zhang, Y. (2016). Being observed magnifies action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 852–865. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000065
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.),
Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing
intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Dueling duelists: Commentary on Carpendale, Atwood,
and Kettner. Human Development, 56, 401–405. https://doi.org/10.1159/000357237
Valloton, C. D., Decker, K. B., Kwon, A., Wang, W., Chang, T. (2016). Quantity and quality of
gestural input: Caregivers’ sensitivity predicts caregiver–infant bidirectional communication through gestures. Infancy, 22(1), 56-77. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12155
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wallbridge, B. (2022). The development of object-extension gestures: An action-based approach
[Unpublished dissertation]. Simon Fraser University.
Wartella, E., Beaudoin-Ryan, L., Blackwell, C. K., Cingel, D. P., Hurwitz, L. B., and Lauricella,
A. R. (2016). What kind of adults will our children become? The impact of growing up in a media-saturated world. Journal of Children Media, 10, 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2015.1124796
Weinberg, M. K., Beeghly, M., Olson, K. L., & Tronick, E. (2008). Effects of maternal
depression and panic disorder on mother-infant interactive behavior in the face-to-face still-face paradigm. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29(5), 472–491. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1002/imhj.20193