The Influence of Absences of Caregiver Responsiveness in Social Interactions on the Development of Infant Social Bids

Main Article Content

Jacob Angeles

Abstract

Social bids are any social behaviour an infant uses to capture their caregiver’s attention to engage in a social activity. Researchers have shown that infants learn to expect caregivers to respond to their social bids and that they may re-attempt a social bid when they do not receive the expected caregiver response. However, the influence of these moments of non-caregiver responsiveness on infant social bid development has been overlooked. In this thesis, I provide a longitudinal description that traces how these moments influence the diversity and amount of the social bids of two caregiver-infant dyads. I found, in line with past research, that infants will re-attempt the same or different social bid when their caregiver is unresponsive. To add, I found that infants increase the diversity and amount of their social bidding over time due to the events of caregiver non-responsiveness. Lastly, infants demonstrated highly idiosyncratic differences in their social bid presentations and changes in relation to caregiver non-responsiveness. These findings suggest that moments of caregiver non-responsiveness may play an important role in the development of infants’ social bid development, which predates their future language and social development.

Article Details

Section
Presentations: Bonds, Identities, and Selves

References

Albert, R. R., Schwade, J. A., & Goldstein, M. H. (2018). The social function of babbling:

Acoustic and contextual characteristics that facilitate maternal responsiveness. Developmental Science, 18(11). https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12641

Ardiel, E. L., & Rankin, C. H. (2010). The importance of touch in development. Paediatrics &

Child Health, 15(3), 153–156. https://doi.org/10.1093/pch/15.3.153

Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and

gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 1004. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.1004

Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech.

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205-226. (Reprinted in E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin, Eds., Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press, 1979, 111-128); reprinted in A. Kasher, Ed., Pragmatics: Critical concepts. London: Routledge, 1998, 274-295.

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of

symbols: Communication and cognition in infancy. New York, NY: Academic.

Butterworth, G. E., & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in

infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14, 219–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646839608404519

Bigelow, A. E., Power, M., Bulmer, M., & Gerrior, K. (2018). The effect of maternal mirroring

behavior on infants’ early social bidding during the still‐face task. Infancy, 23(3), 367–385. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1111/infa.12221

Bigelow, A. E., Power, M. (2022). Influences of infants’ and mothers’ contingent vocal

responsiveness on young infants’ vocal social bids in the Still Face Task. Infant Behavior and Development, 69, 101776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101776.

Blake, J., O’Rourke, P., & Borzellino, G. (1994). Form and function in the development of

pointing and reaching gestures. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 195-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(94)90055-8

Boundy, L., Cameron-Faulkner, T., & Theakston, A. (2016). Exploring early communicative

behaviours: A fine-grained analysis of infant shows and gives. Infant Behavior & Development, 44, 86–97. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.005

Boundy, L., Cameron, F. T., & Theakston, A. (2019). Intention or attention before pointing: Do

infants’ early holdout gestures reflect evidence of a declarative motive? Infancy, 24(2), 228–248. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1111/infa.12267

Canfield, J. V. (1995). The rudiments of language. Language & Communication, 15(3), 195–211.

https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(95)00006-C

Carpendale, J. I. M., Atwood, S., & Kettner, V., (2013). Meaning and mind from the perspective

of dualist versus relational worldviews: Implications for the development of pointing gestures. Human Development, 56, 381–400. https://doi.org/10.1159/000357235

Carpendale, J. I. M., & Carpendale, A. B. (2010). The development of pointing: From personal

directedness to interpersonal direction. Human Development, 53, 110-126. https://doi.org/10.1159/000315168

Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The

development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 79–151. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000032

Carpenter, M. (2009). Just how joint is joint action in infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2),

–392. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01026.x

Cameron-Faulkner T, Theakston A, Lieven E, & Tomasello M (2015). The relationship between

infant holdout and gives, and pointing. Infancy, 20(5), 576–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12085

Cavell, T. A., & Quetsch, L. B. (2022). Good enough parenting: A six-point plan for a

stronger relationship with your child. American Psychological Association. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1037/0000324-000

Choi, B., Shah, P., Rowe, M. L., Nelson, C. A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2020). Gesture

development, caregiver responsiveness, and language and diagnostic outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 50(7), 2556–2572. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03980-8

Church, Sarah, Michael Dunn, and Linda Prokopy. 2019. Benefits to qualitative data quality

with multiple coders: Two case studies in multi-coder data analysis. Journal of Rural Social Sciences, 34(1). Article 2.

Collis, G. (1979). Describing the structure of social interaction in infancy. In M. Bullowa (Ed.),

Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for

developing grounded theory (3rd ed.). Sage Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452230153

Dahl, A. (2017). Ecological commitments: Why developmental science needs naturalistic

methods. Child Development Perspectives, 11(2), 79–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12217

Danziger, K. (1985). The methodological imperative in psychology. Philosophy of Social

Sciences, 15, 1–13.

Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In

C. Moore, & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

Esseily, R., Jacquet, A. Y., & Fagard, J. (2011). Handedness for grasping objects and pointing

and the development of language in 14‐month‐old infants. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 16, 565–585. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.499911

Fusaro, M. (2010). Toddlers get the nod: An analysis of “yes” and “no” gesture use during the

second to third year of life (Publication No. 3427125) [Doctural dissertation]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global.

Fusaro, M., Vallotton, C. D., & Harris, P. L. (2014). Beside the point: Mothers’ head nodding and

shaking gestures during parent–child play. Infant Behavior & Development, 37(2), 235–247. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.01.006

Fusaro, M., & Vallotton, C. D. (2011). Giving a nod to social cognition: Developmental

constraints on the emergence of conventional gestures and infant signs. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 4, 121-135. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1075/gs.4.11fus

Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H.M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian

learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1085–1108. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028044

Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveria, E. (2015).

Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1167. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167

Gros‐Louis, J., West, M. J., & King, A. P. (2014). Maternal responsiveness and the development

of directed vocalizing in social interactions. Infancy, 19(4), 385–408. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12054

Harding, C. G., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in

prelinguistic infants. Child Development, 50, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129038

Hsu, H.-C., Fogel, A., & Messinger, D. S. (2001). Infant non-distress vocalization during

mother-infant face-to-face interaction: Factors associated with quantitative and qualitative differences. Infant Behavior & Development, 24(1), 107–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00061-3

Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in

infancy: Coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(2). https://doi.org/10.1037/pap0000176

Jopling, D. (1993). Cognitive science, other minds, and the philosophy of dialogue. In U. Neisser

(Ed.), The perceived self (pp. 290–309). Cambridge: MIT Press.

O’Donnell, James J. “Augustine’s Classical Readings.” Recherches Augustiniennes, 1980,

–75. https://doi.org/10.5840/ancientphil199212239

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental

disorders. Trends Cognitive Science, 2, 389–398. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01230-3

Kettner, V.A., & Carpendale, J.I.M. (2018). From touching to communicating: Forms of index

finger use in the development of pointing. Gesture, 17(2), 245-268. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18005.ket

Kishimoto, T. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal observations of pointing gestures by

infants and their caregivers in Japan. Cognitive Development, 43, 235-244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.06.001

Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). Infants’ joint attention skills predict

toddlers’ emerging mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1207–1219. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1037/a0024808

Kwon, A. Y., Vallotton, C. D., Kiegelmann, M., & Wilhelm, K. H., (2018). Cultural

diversification of communicative gestures through early childhood: A comparison of children in English- , German-, and Chinese- speaking families. Infant Behavior and Development, 50, 328-339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.10.003

Berends, L., & Johnston, J. (2005) Using multiple coders to enhance qualitative

analysis: The case of interviews with consumers of drug treatment. Addiction Research & Theory, 13(4), 373-381. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066350500102237

Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C. (2012). A prelinguistic

gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x

Locke LM, Prinz RJ. Measurement of parental discipline and nurturance. Clinical Psychology

Review, 22, 895–929. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00133-2

Masataka, N. (2003). From index-finger extension to index-finger pointing: Ontogenesis of

pointing in preverbal infants. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 69-84). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Matsuki, T., Ebara, T., Tamada, H., Kato, S., Kaneko, K., Kano, H., Matsuzaki, K., Sato, H.,

Minato, K., Sugiura-Ogasawara, M., Saitoh, S., & Kamijima, M. (2022). Repeated maternal non-responsiveness to baby’s crying during postpartum and infant neuropsychological development: The Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 127, 1–15. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1016/j.chiabu.2022.105581

Meeus, W. (2016). Adolescent psychosocial development: A review of longitudinal models and

research. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1969–1993. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000243

Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of

the Still-Face Paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29(2), 120–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001

Mcquaid, N. E., Bibok, M. B., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2009). Relation between maternal

contingent responsiveness and infant social expectations. Infancy, 14(3), 390–401. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000902839955

Moreno-Núøez A, Rodríguez C, & Miranda-Zapata E (2020). Getting away from the point: The

emergence of ostensive gestures and their functions. Journal of Child Language, 47(3), 556–578. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000606

Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007).

Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78(3), 938–954. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x

Onishi, K., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science,

, 255– 258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621

Özçalışkan, Ş., Adamson, L. B., & Dimitrova, N. (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures

predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism, 20, 754–763. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361315605921

Plato. (1901). Phaedo (B. Jowett, Trans.). In Plato & B. Jowett (Trans.), Dialogues of Plato: With

analyses and introductions. Charles Scribner's Sons. https://doi.org/10.1037/13728-011

Provenzi, L., Giusti, L., & Montirosso, R. (2016). Do infants exhibit significant cortisol

reactivity to the Face-to-Face Still-Face paradigm? A narrative review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 42, 34–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.07.001

Reddy, V. (2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 397–402. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00191-8

Reddy, V. (2011). A gaze at grips with me. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New

developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience. MIT Press.

Reddy, V. (2018). Why Engagement? A Second-Person Take on Social Cognition. In A. Newen,

L. de Bruin, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Rips, L. J. (2002). Circular reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26(6), 767–795.

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0364-0213(02)00085-X

Rowe, M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low‐income caregivers and their 14‐month‐old

children. First Language, 20, 305–330. https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237000200600

Rowe, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in

child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951-953. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167025

Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U. (2013). Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of

prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development, 84(4), 1296-1307. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12026

Scaife, M. & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature,

, 265-266. https://doi.org/10.1038/253265a0

Steinmetz, J., Xu, Q., Fishbach, A., & Zhang, Y. (2016). Being observed magnifies action.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(6), 852–865. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000065

Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.),

Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing

intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129

Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Dueling duelists: Commentary on Carpendale, Atwood,

and Kettner. Human Development, 56, 401–405. https://doi.org/10.1159/000357237

Valloton, C. D., Decker, K. B., Kwon, A., Wang, W., Chang, T. (2016). Quantity and quality of

gestural input: Caregivers’ sensitivity predicts caregiver–infant bidirectional communication through gestures. Infancy, 22(1), 56-77. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12155

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

Wallbridge, B. (2022). The development of object-extension gestures: An action-based approach

[Unpublished dissertation]. Simon Fraser University.

Wartella, E., Beaudoin-Ryan, L., Blackwell, C. K., Cingel, D. P., Hurwitz, L. B., and Lauricella,

A. R. (2016). What kind of adults will our children become? The impact of growing up in a media-saturated world. Journal of Children Media, 10, 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2015.1124796

Weinberg, M. K., Beeghly, M., Olson, K. L., & Tronick, E. (2008). Effects of maternal

depression and panic disorder on mother-infant interactive behavior in the face-to-face still-face paradigm. Infant Mental Health Journal, 29(5), 472–491. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1002/imhj.20193